- Department of Neurosurgery, Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
Correspondence Address:
Ahmad Hafez
Department of Neurosurgery, Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
DOI:10.4103/sni.sni_427_17
Copyright: © 2018 Surgical Neurology International This is an open access journal, and articles are distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 License, which allows others to remix, tweak, and build upon the work non-commercially, as long as appropriate credit is given and the new creations are licensed under the identical terms.How to cite this article: Antti Huotarinen, Mika Niemelä, Ahmad Hafez. The impact of neurosurgical procedure on cognitive resources: Results of bypass training. 05-Apr-2018;9:71
How to cite this URL: Antti Huotarinen, Mika Niemelä, Ahmad Hafez. The impact of neurosurgical procedure on cognitive resources: Results of bypass training. 05-Apr-2018;9:71. Available from: http://surgicalneurologyint.com/?post_type=surgicalint_articles&p=8827
Abstract
Background:Neurosurgeons are exposed to unavoidable distractions in their natural operating environment. Distractions can affect both the surgeon's concentration and the safety and duration of the surgery. Such distraction can be studied by applying a simultaneous cognitive task during a surgical procedure.
Methods:We used a previously described cognitive task: a forward (DF) and backward digit (DB) repetition task to interfere with the surgeon's attention during a training bypass. A pilot study was performed to find suitable digit repetition lengths. For the main experiment, we used four-digit strings. The test task was alternated across two consecutive sutures (n = 153, 8 bypasses), followed by two consecutive control sutures without digit repetition. The duration and the number of correct answers for the digit repetition task were compared to a baseline digit repetition without simultaneous surgery.
Results:During the bypass surgery, digit repetitions (especially DB) became slower (P P P = 0.823).
Conclusions:The ability to engage in simultaneous tasks while performing surgery is diminished. A surgeon with extensive training can withstand external distraction without an effect on performance; however, this is achieved by partially ignoring the simultaneous task. Our data support that during surgery other cognitive tasks should be avoided to ensure safety.
Keywords: Bypass surgery, cognitive distraction, neurosurgery
INTRODUCTION
During surgery neurosurgeons are exposed to different kinds of distraction, such as operating room (OR) staff and equipment movement, questions from observers, residents, and others, and phone consulting. Several authors have studied the effects of distraction and interruption during clinical practice in many fields of healthcare, especially in the OR.[
Bypass surgery is a highly complex skill with high time pressure. Mastering it requires both extensive practice and a strong ability to concentrate, i.e., sustain focused attention during the actual procedure. Attention and working memory have been shown to be closely related processes.[
Repetition of auditory cued digit strings of various lengths is one method of measuring working memory capacity and is used as a central part of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), designed by psychologist David Wechsler and first published in 1955. The normal lengths for forward (DF) and backward (DB) repetition of digit strings are described for various populations;[
Our aim was to develop and test a method based on previously simulated training models as well as psychological studies that examine controllable and quantifiable cognitive loading during microsurgical training, and to study cognitive requirements and distractibility during bypass surgery. Our hypothesis was that even trained surgeons are susceptible to outside cognitive distractions while performing complex tasks such as bypass training, and that multitasking with digit repetition (both DF and DB) can impair bypass performance, and that the digit string memory tasks are significantly affected by simultaneous bypass suturing.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
In this experimental study, all training bypass procedures were end-to-side procedures, simulating the most common superficial temporal artery to medial cerebral artery (STA-MCA) bypass procedure. It was performed by the last author (Ahmad Hafez), who performed 1,300 different training bypass procedures over the past 3 years, between June 2014 and July 2017, in the Department of Neurosurgery, Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland.
Study design
The main rationale for the experiment was to compare individual suture times during multitasking with digit repetition (both DF and DB) to individual suture times without a digit repetition task. For this purpose, we designed experiments wherein the digit repetition task and the control (no repetition task) were alternated over one or two consecutive sutures during training bypass procedures.
Pilot 1
Pilot 1 was a feasibility experiment designed to find the range of string-repetition lengths possible during suturing under the microscope.
Digit strings were read aloud by the researcher, without a pause after the answer. Each digit repetition task lasted for one interrupted suture. Digit repetition lengths were 3, 5, and 7 digits for the forward repetition task, and 3 and 5 digits for the backward repetition task [
In this pilot, the procedure was a single end-to-side bypass on a 1 mm silicone wet tube with 10-0 sutures. The bypass was recorded on video and the times for individual bypasses were analyzed from the video.
Pilot 2
Pilot 2 was designed to confirm that the chosen four-digit string forward (4-DF) and backward (4-DB) repetition tasks were suitable for the actual experiment.
The four-digit strings were read by a synthesized female voice at one digit per se cond with a 6-second pause between each four-digit string to allow time for answering. The 4-DB repetitions were tested with and without surgery and compared against a control task of 4-DB repetitions without surgery.
Main experiment
In the main experiment, there were three groups of bypasses: control without any digit repetition tasks (n = 2), bypass with simultaneous 4-DF task (n = 2), and bypass with simultaneous 4-DB task (n = 3) [
Figure 1
Study design for the main experiment. (a) Top: Bypass suturing scheme showing the first four hanging sutures (black) and sutures on each side included in the digit repetition analysis (red), two consecutive sutures (dotted circles). Middle: Outline of sutures during bypasses with digit repetition task and control sutures without digit repetition tasks in the main experiment. During each individual bypass, the task was four-digit repetition either forward or backward. Bottom: Organization of the four-digit repetition task baselines without simultaneous bypass (digit repetition only), digit repetition bypasses (DF and DB), and control bypasses during the main experiment. (b) Instrumentation for the bypass and video capture. (c) A screenshot of wall piercing during suturing in one of the bypasses. (d) Completed bypass just prior to removing the temporary clips
Surgery
Surgery was done with an OPMI pico tabletop microscope (Zeiss, Jena, Germany) [
Video capture and analysis
Webcam C930e (Logitech, Lausanne, Switzerland) was connected to the microscope and connected to a MacBook Pro (Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA) that captured video with QuickTime (Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA) at 720p video quality. Video analysis was done on Final Cut Pro X (Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA) at 50 frames per second.
Suturing times were measured from video. The time needed to complete a single suture was measured from picking up the needle to picking up the needle before the following suture. We also recorded the times for each suture's subcomponents: needle pick up, wall piercing [
Digit repetition analysis
All digit repetition analysis was performed from recorded audio separately from the video analysis. We measured the time for the answer as the interval between the last audible part of the final digit of the task string and the last audible part of the final digit of the answer. The correctness of answers was measured from the recorded audio. The number of correct digits in each answer string was scored individually.
Statistics
All statistical comparisons between multiple groups were done with one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for parametric variables and with Kruskal–Wallis for nonparametric variables. All analysis was done using Prism 7.0b (GraphPad, San Diego, USA) software.
RESULTS
Pilot 1
Only 5-DB digit repetition times were significantly longer than the baseline 3-DF repetition during the control task (no surgery) (ANOVA F[9,67] =20.5, P < 0.0001, Dunnett's multiple comparison, P < 0.0001). During the surgical bypass task, the digit repetition times were longer than baseline for the 7-DF repetition (Dunnett's multiple comparison, P = 0.028), 3-DB repetition (Dunnett's multiple comparison, P = 0.0001), and 5-DB repetition (Dunnett's multiple comparison, P = 0.0001) [
Figure 2
(a) Pilot 1 results showing the increase in the duration of digit repetition times as digits per second for DF and DB repetition and different digit string lengths during control (no surgery) and bypass training surgery. (b) Pilot 2 results of digit repetition times for digit repetition times without simultaneous bypass training (control forward and control backward) and during bypass training (bypass backward). Error bars represent SD, DF, DB
Pilot 2
Answer time was significantly longer during surgery with 4-DB compared to 4-DF without surgery (ANOVA F[2,20] =3.862, P = 0.038, post hoc Tukey P = 0.0366) [
Main experiment
The average time for a completed bypass [
Figure 3
(a) Bypass times during the main experiment showing no difference between study groups. (b) Individual suture times during the main experiment separated for the bypass (control or dual task), task type during the bypass (control, DF, or DB) and first four sutures (BL) showing no differences between study groups. (c) Answer durations during the main experiment for digit repetition without simultaneous bypass training (control DF and DB) and digit repetition times during simultaneous bypass training (bypass DF and DB). (d) The average number of correct digits produced during without simultaneous bypass training, during bypass, and only during bypass is the number of correct answers decreased Error bars represent SD; *P < 0.005, **P < 0.0001; DF, digit repeat forward; DB, digit repeat backward
Average time for individual side sutures (non-hanging sutures) was 45.9 s (SD 13.2). There was no difference between the time needed for individual sutures between study groups (ANOVA F[4,116] =0.163, P = 0.956). Baseline hanging sutures were slower than side sutures in all study groups (all t-test P < 0.025) [
Digit repetition
During the bypass, the repetition of four-digit strings was slower for both 4-DF and 4-DB repetitions than for similar repetitions without bypass. In addition, the 4-DB were slower compared to the 4-DF, both without the bypass and during the bypass [
DISCUSSION
This study focused on the effects of distraction during surgery, which occurs frequently in neurosurgical practice. It is the first study we are aware of that quantifies and evaluates the effects of simultaneous cognitive tasks during one of the most complex procedures in neurosurgery—the bypass procedure.
We demonstrated that simultaneous cognitive distraction had minimal or no effect on surgical performance; this could be explained by the shifting of attention to the most relevant task, the bypass. However, the bypass task had a significant effect on the digit repetition task, as shown by the increase in time needed to answer and the decrease in the number of correct answers. This implies that the solution to retaining adequate mental resources needed for the bypass is partial or total neglect of the other mental task.
Allocation of limited cognitive resources (i.e., attention) remains a major factor during neurosurgical procedures. Multitasking, which is an attempt to perform two or more tasks simultaneously, is an important ability for the neurosurgeon. However, during multitasking, more mistakes are made or performance is slower as the surgeon's attention is distributed among the tasks.[
The expert neurosurgeon relies on reflexive manual maneuvering and sustained focus achieved by overlearning the skill so that it becomes automated and not easily affected by outside interference, as is also seen in this study. However, this automatization requires extended training, especially for the bypass procedures. Novice neurosurgeons have no opportunity to master bypass surgery through real surgery because of its infrequent use in clinical practice.[
Recent advances in virtual reality technology can enhance surgical training and provide the opportunity for detailed feedback and performance evaluation. It is not far in the future that detailed virtual reality neurosurgical modules will evolve to be a part of training programs in neurosurgery.[
This study was conducted without risk of harm to patients since it examined cognitive distraction in the laboratory, during training bypass surgery. The study provided information on the cognitive requirements of bypass surgery and can predict some issues regarding the safety of neurosurgical practice in general. It also connects the literature on neurosurgical training to psychological and cognitive literature, which can help us further deepen our understanding of the cognitive and attentional requirements of neurosurgical operations in general.
Our data show that digit repetition performance was worse during practice bypass compared to the control task without surgery, showing a quantifiable cognitive cost of bypass surgery. However, a simultaneous digit repetition task during bypass did not have an effect on bypass performance, as measured by the time needed to complete individual sutures; this might be explained by a capacity to increase overall attentional control during multitasking or the tendency to ignore the secondary cognitive task during bypass. It is also possible that the cognitive task used in this study was not sufficiently challenging. However, in our pilot study, we found that longer backward digit repetitions were not feasible. It is also possible that the distracting cognitive task should have been of a different type to have a negative effect on bypass performance. However, the capacity to perform the simultaneous cognitive task was very clearly affected, which suggests that the backward digit repetition process in particular relies on some of the same cognitive and attentional processes that are required for bypass surgery. In addition, it has been recently described that attentional control can either improve or deteriorate under pressure in an experimental setting.[
Limitations
The main limitation is that this study includes only a single surgeon, so the results might reflect in part his personal traits and the very high volume of bypass training he underwent in previous years. Although it is an attractive study with important findings, it is less realistic than naturalistic observation; however, such observation is difficult to apply during clinical neurosurgery due to safety concerns for the patient. Although all bypasses were inspected to ensure equal quality, this parameter was not discussed, as it did not affect the main scope of the study. However, we are in the process of preparing many studies focusing on training quality in neurosurgery.
Conclusions
We conclude that the ability to engage in a secondary task while operating is diminished, especially when the secondary task becomes more complex. The presence of simultaneous distraction, especially during a highly demanding task, could negatively impact the automaticity of surgical performance. Time and intensity of distraction, as well as the experience of the neurosurgeon, can all affect the outcome of the surgery. For the patient's safety and excellent neurosurgical outcome, it is important to recognize the type and the level of distraction and interruption with which an individual neurosurgeon can cope. Our data recommend that extra cognitive tasks should be avoided during surgery to ensure safety.
Financial support and sponsorship
Nil.
Conflicts of interest
There are no conflicts of interest.
References
1. Aboud E, Aboud G, Al-Mefty O, Aboud T, Rammos S, Abolfotoh M. “Live cadavers” for training in the management of intraoperative aneurysmal rupture. J Neurosurg. 2015. 123: 1339-46
2. Aboud E, Al-Mefty O, Yasargil MG. New laboratory model for neurosurgical training that simulates live surgery. J Neurosurg. 2002. 97: 1367-72
3. Alaraj A, Lemole MG, Finkle JH, Yudkowsky R, Wallace A, Luciano C. Virtual reality training in neurosurgery: Review of current status and future applications. Surg Neurol Int. 2011. 2: 52-
4. Anderson DA, Burton DB, Parker JD, Godding PR. A confirmatory factor analysis of the cognitive capacity screening examination in a clinical sample. Int J Neurosci. 2001. 111: 221-33
5. Awh E, Vogel EK, Oh SH. Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience. 2006. 139: 201-8
6. Baddeley A. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003. 4: 829-839
7. Brown LA. Spatial-Sequential Working Memory in Younger and Older Adults: Age Predicts Backward Recall Performance within Both Age Groups. Front Psychol. 2016. 7: 1514-
8. Campbell G, Arfanis K, Smith AF. Distraction and interruption in anaesthetic practice. Br J Anaesth. 2012. 109: 707-15
9. Choudhury N, Gelinas-Phaneuf N, Delorme S, Del Maestro R. Fundamentals of neurosurgery: Virtual reality tasks for training and evaluation of technical skills. World Neurosurg. 2013. 80: e9-19
10. Eivazi S, Hafez A, Fuhl W, Afkari H, Kasneci E, Lehecka M. Optimal eye movement strategies: A comparison of neurosurgeons gaze patterns when using a surgical microscope. Acta Neurochir (Wien). 2017. 159: 959-66
11. Enser M, Moriceau J, Abily J, Damm C, Occhiali E, Besnier E. Background noise lowers the performance of anaesthesiology residents’ clinical reasoning when measured by script concordance: A randomised crossover volunteer study. Eur J Anaesthesiol. 2017. 34: 464-70
12. Gerton BK, Brown TT, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn P, Holt JL, Olsen RK. Shared and distinct neurophysiological components of the digits forward and backward tasks as revealed by functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia. 2004. 42: 1781-7
13. Hawksworth C, Asbury AJ, Millar K. Music in theatre: Not so harmonious.A survey of attitudes to music played in the operating theatre. Anaesthesia. 1997. 52: 79-83
14. Healey AN, Sevdalis N, Vincent CA. Measuring intra-operative interference from distraction and interruption observed in the operating theatre. Ergonomics. 2006. 49: 589-604
15. Hernesniemi J, Niemela M, Karatas A, Kivipelto L, Ishii K, Rinne J. Some collected principles of microneurosurgery: Simple and fast, while preserving normal anatomy: A review. Surg Neurol. 2005. 64: 195-200
16. Higurashi M, Qian Y, Zecca M, Park YK, Umezu M, Morgan MK. Surgical training technology for cerebrovascular anastomosis. J Clin Neurosci. 2014. 21: 554-8
17. Hino A. Training in microvascular surgery using a chicken wing artery. Neurosurgery. 2003. 52: 1495-7
18. Hinson JM, Jameson TL, Whitney P. Somatic markers, working memory, and decision making. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2002. 2: 341-53
19. Hoshi Y, Oda I, Wada Y, Ito Y, Yutaka Y, Oda M. Visuospatial imagery is a fruitful strategy for the digit span backward task: A study with near-infrared optical tomography. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2000. 9: 339-42
20. Hsu KE, Man FY, Gizicki RA, Feldman LS, Fried GM. Experienced surgeons can do more than one thing at a time: Effect of distraction on performance of a simple laparoscopic and cognitive task by experienced and novice surgeons. Surg Endosc. 2008. 22: 196-201
21. Indo M, Tsutsumi K, Shin M. The practice of knots untying technique using a 10-0 nylon suture and gauze to cope with technical difficulties of microvascular anastomosis. World Neurosurg. 2011. 75: 87-9
22. Kracht JM, Busch-Vishniac IJ, West JE. Noise in the operating rooms of Johns Hopkins Hospital. J Acoust Soc Am. 2007. 121: 2673-80
23. Lipsman N, Khan O, Kulkarni AV. “The Actualized Neurosurgeon”: A Proposed Model of Surgical Resident Development. World Neurosurg. 2017. 99: 381-6
24. Luo X, Zhang L, Wang J. The Benefits of Working Memory Capacity on Attentional Control under Pressure. Front Psychol. 2017. 8: 1105-
25. Meier ME, Smeekens BA, Silvia PJ, Kwapil TR, Kane MJ. Working Memory Capacity and the Antisaccade Task: A Microanalytic-Macroanalytic Investigation of Individual Differences in Goal Activation and Maintenance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2018. 44: 68-84
26. Mentis HM, Chellali A, Manser K, Cao CG, Schwaitzberg SD. A systematic review of the effect of distraction on surgeon performance: Directions for operating room policy and surgical training. Surg Endosc. 2016. 30: 1713-24
27. Poplu G, Ripoll H, Mavromatis S, Baratgin J. How do expert soccer players encode visual information to make decisions in simulated game situations?. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2008. 79: 392-8
28. Primus CP, Healey AN, Undre S. Distraction in the urology operating theatre. BJU Int. 2007. 99: 493-4
29. Rehder R, Abd-El-Barr M, Hooten K, Weinstock P, Madsen JR, Cohen AR. The role of simulation in neurosurgery. Childs Nerv Syst. 2016. 32: 43-54
30. Rivera-Rodriguez AJ, Karsh BT. Interruptions and distractions in healthcare: Review and reappraisal. Qual Saf Health Care. 2010. 19: 304-12
31. Russin J, Giannotta SL. The value of translational models for microvascular anastamosis. World Neurosurg. 2012. 77: 289-90
32. Ryherd EE, Waye KP, Ljungkvist L. Characterizing noise and perceived work environment in a neurological intensive care unit. J Acoust Soc Am. 2008. 123: 747-56
33. Ryu J, Choi SK, Chung Y, Lee SH, Jeong BO. A Portable Training Model for Deep Bypass Surgery. World Neurosurg. 2017. 107: 263-7
34. Seidler RD, Bo J, Anguera JA. Neurocognitive contributions to motor skill learning: The role of working memory. J Motor Behav. 2012. 44: 445-53
35. Sevdalis N, Healey AN, Vincent CA. Distracting communications in the operating theatre. J Eval Clin Pract. 2007. 13: 390-4
36. Smith AF, Mahajan RP. National critical incident reporting: Improving patient safety. Br J Anaesth. 2009. 103: 623-5
37. Unsworth N, Schrock JC, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and the antisaccade task: Individual differences in voluntary saccade control. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004. 30: 1302-21
38. Wahn B, Konig P. Is Attentional Resource Allocation Across Sensory Modalities Task-Dependent?. Adv Cogn Psychol. 2017. 13: 83-96
39. Wanzel KR, Hamstra SJ, Anastakis DJ, Matsumoto ED, Cusimano MD. Effect of visual-spatial ability on learning of spatially-complex surgical skills. Lancet. 2002. 359: 230-1
40. Yoong W, Khin A, Ramlal N, Loabile B, Forman S. Interruptions and distractions in the gynaecological operating theatre: Irritating or dangerous?. Ergonomics. 2015. 58: 1314-9